Causal Claim Definition: A Simple Guide With Examples!

Causal claim definition is a crucial concept, influencing decisions across diverse fields. Researchers use statistical analysis to rigorously evaluate the validity of these claims. Philosophers, from Aristotle onward, have debated the underlying nature of causation, contributing significantly to causal claim definition. For practical application, a deep understanding of regression analysis is often indispensable in establishing and testing claims. This guide offers a simplified breakdown of causal claim definition and illustrative examples, equipping you with the tools to navigate these complex arguments.

In a world brimming with information, the ability to discern genuine cause-and-effect relationships is more critical than ever. From shaping public policy to informing personal decisions, causal claims underpin our understanding of how the world works. Yet, not all statements are created equal.

Table of Contents

Differentiating Causal Claims from Other Assertions

It’s easy to mistake a simple observation or a prediction for a causal claim. A causal claim goes beyond merely stating that two things are related; it asserts that one thing directly influences or produces another.

Consider the statement, "People who exercise regularly tend to be healthier." This is an observation, perhaps even a correlation.

However, the causal claim would be, "Regular exercise causes improved health outcomes." The difference lies in the assertion of a direct influence, a mechanism by which one action leads to a specific result.

Distinguishing between these types of statements is paramount to avoiding flawed reasoning and making sound judgments. One must analyze if one action truly causes the other, or if there is another explanation for the phenomenon.

Thesis: Navigating the Labyrinth of Causality

This guide aims to equip you with the tools necessary to navigate the complex landscape of causality. We will provide a clear causal claim definition, carefully differentiating it from the often-confused concept of correlation.

Furthermore, we will explore the common pitfalls that can lead to false causal inferences.

By understanding these distinctions and potential errors, you’ll be better prepared to critically evaluate claims and make informed decisions based on solid evidence and logical reasoning.

In discerning causality, we’ve highlighted the importance of understanding the difference between a mere correlation and a true causal relationship. This distinction is crucial, and it all begins with a clear definition of what exactly constitutes a causal claim.

Defining Causal Claims: What’s the Core Meaning?

At its heart, a causal claim is a statement that asserts a direct relationship between two events, actions, or variables.

It goes beyond simply observing that two things occur together; it postulates that one is responsible for the other.

In other words, a causal claim argues that one thing causes another.

The Essence of Cause and Effect

The relationship between cause and effect is the cornerstone of a causal claim.

The cause is the initial event or action that sets in motion a chain of events.

The effect is the resulting outcome or consequence.

For a claim to be considered causal, there must be a demonstrable connection between the two.

This connection implies that the cause directly influences or produces the effect.

Asserting Direct Influence

It’s crucial to emphasize that a causal claim doesn’t just suggest a connection.

It asserts that one thing directly brings about another.

This assertion is what separates a causal claim from a statement of correlation or association.

Consider the difference between saying "People who study more tend to get better grades" and "Studying more causes people to get better grades."

The first statement observes a relationship, while the second asserts a direct influence.

Key Components of a Causal Claim

Every causal claim, regardless of its complexity, comprises two essential elements: the proposed cause and the resulting effect.

Understanding these components is vital for analyzing and evaluating causal arguments.

The Proposed Cause: The Independent Variable

The proposed cause is often referred to as the independent variable.

This is the factor that is believed to influence or produce a particular outcome.

In an experimental setting, the independent variable is the one that researchers manipulate to observe its effect on another variable.

Identifying the independent variable is crucial for understanding the basis of the causal claim.

The Resulting Effect: The Dependent Variable

The resulting effect is frequently termed the dependent variable.

This is the outcome or consequence that is believed to be influenced by the independent variable.

The dependent variable is what researchers measure to determine whether the independent variable had a significant impact.

Recognizing the dependent variable is essential for assessing the validity of the causal claim.

In discerning causality, we’ve highlighted the importance of understanding the difference between a mere correlation and a true causal relationship. This distinction is crucial, and it all begins with a clear definition of what exactly constitutes a causal claim.

Once you’ve grasped the essence of causal claims, a new challenge emerges: differentiating them from simple correlations. This is where many well-intentioned analyses go astray, leading to flawed conclusions and misguided actions. Let’s delve into why correlation does not equal causation, and how to avoid this common pitfall.

Causation vs. Correlation: Untangling the Misconception

One of the most persistent and pervasive errors in reasoning is the assumption that correlation implies causation. Just because two things happen to occur together, or even move in tandem, doesn’t automatically mean that one causes the other. Understanding this distinction is absolutely vital for sound decision-making and avoiding costly mistakes.

The Crucial Distinction: Correlation Does Not Equal Causation

Correlation simply describes a statistical relationship between two variables. When one variable changes, the other tends to change as well. This relationship can be positive (both variables increase together) or negative (as one variable increases, the other decreases).

However, this co-occurrence doesn’t tell us why the variables are moving together.

It doesn’t tell us if one is directly influencing the other.

Defining Correlation

At its core, correlation is a statistical measure that quantifies the extent to which two or more variables are related. It indicates the strength and direction of a linear relationship between them. A correlation coefficient, typically ranging from -1 to +1, summarizes this relationship.

A coefficient of +1 indicates a perfect positive correlation, meaning the variables increase or decrease together perfectly.

A coefficient of -1 indicates a perfect negative correlation, meaning one variable increases as the other decreases perfectly.

A coefficient of 0 indicates no linear correlation.

Examples of Correlated Events That Are Not Causally Related

Consider the classic example of the number of firefighters sent to a fire and the amount of damage caused by the fire. There’s likely a positive correlation: more firefighters, more damage.

Does this mean that firefighters cause more damage?

Of course not! Both the number of firefighters and the amount of damage are likely caused by a third factor: the size and intensity of the fire itself.

Here’s another example: mobile phone use is correlated with increased lifespan. That does not mean mobile phones are magic life extending devices. The real explanation is that mobile phone usage is correlated with wealth, and the wealthy have access to better healthcare.

These examples highlight that correlation can be a misleading indicator of causation.

Spurious Correlation: The Illusion of Causality

Spurious correlation takes the deception of correlation one step further. It occurs when two variables appear to be related, but their correlation is actually due to a lurking variable – a third, unmeasured variable that influences both.

This creates an illusory relationship between the original two, making it seem like they are causally connected when they are not.

Ice Cream Sales and Crime Rates: A Classic Example

A well-known example of spurious correlation is the observed relationship between ice cream sales and crime rates. Studies have shown that as ice cream sales increase, so does the rate of crime.

Does this mean that ice cream consumption leads to criminal behavior?

Probably not.

The more likely explanation is that both ice cream sales and crime rates tend to increase during the summer months. Higher temperatures encourage people to buy ice cream and also create conditions that may contribute to increased crime (e.g., more people outside, longer daylight hours).

Temperature, in this case, is the lurking variable that creates the spurious correlation.

Why It Matters: The Dangers of Confusing Correlation with Causation

Treating correlations as causations can lead to serious errors in judgment and decision-making. Imagine a business making investment decisions based on spurious correlations they have found in their data.

For example, a company might notice that sales of a particular product increase whenever they run a specific advertisement. They might conclude that the advertisement causes the increase in sales and invest heavily in that ad campaign.

However, if the increase in sales is actually due to a seasonal trend or a broader marketing campaign running concurrently, the company could be wasting resources on an ineffective advertisement.

In public policy, mistaking correlation for causation can have even more far-reaching consequences. For example, if policymakers observe a correlation between a particular social program and a decrease in crime rates, they might attribute the decrease to the program and expand it.

However, if the decrease in crime rates is actually due to a broader economic trend or a change in policing strategies, the expanded program might not be effective and could divert resources from more impactful interventions.

Therefore, always be careful assuming that correlation equals causation.

Once you’ve sifted through the deceptive allure of mere correlations, the real work begins. Establishing a genuine causal link is not a trivial pursuit; it requires careful consideration of several key criteria. Without adhering to these principles, even the most intuitively appealing causal claim remains suspect.

Establishing Causality: Essential Considerations

To confidently assert that X causes Y, one must move beyond simple observation and engage in rigorous investigation. This involves addressing temporal order, demonstrating covariation, controlling for confounding variables, providing robust evidence, and articulating a plausible mechanism. Each of these elements plays a crucial role in building a strong and defensible causal argument.

Temporal Precedence: First Comes the Cause

The most fundamental requirement for establishing causality is temporal precedence. This simply means that the cause must come before the effect in time. It seems obvious, but it’s a point that’s often overlooked or misinterpreted.

If Y occurs before X, then X cannot possibly be the cause of Y. For example, you cannot claim that a new exercise program caused weight loss if the weight loss occurred before starting the program.

This principle helps to eliminate many potential causal relationships right away, as it provides a clear and unambiguous criterion for assessment.

Covariation: Showing a Relationship

Beyond temporal order, there must also be covariation between the cause and the effect. This means that the cause and effect must be related.

More specifically, when the cause is present, the effect is more likely to occur. And when the cause is absent, the effect is less likely to occur.

This relationship doesn’t need to be perfect (i.e., the effect doesn’t need to always occur when the cause is present). However, there should be a statistically significant association between the two variables.

Eliminating Alternative Explanations: The Challenge of Confounding Variables

Perhaps the most difficult aspect of establishing causality is ruling out alternative explanations. This is where confounding variables come into play.

A confounding variable is a third variable that is related to both the proposed cause and the effect. This variable can create the illusion of a causal relationship when, in fact, it is the confounding variable that is driving the observed association.

For example, imagine that a study finds a correlation between ice cream consumption and sunburns. Does this mean that eating ice cream causes sunburns?

Probably not. The more likely explanation is that a third variable—sunny weather—is causing both increased ice cream consumption and higher rates of sunburn.

Controlling for confounding variables is often achieved through careful experimental design (e.g., randomization) and statistical analysis (e.g., multiple regression).

The Role of Evidence: Building a Strong Case

No causal claim can stand on its own without substantial evidence. This evidence can come from a variety of sources, including:

  • Observational studies: These studies observe the relationship between variables without actively manipulating them.

  • Experimental studies: These studies actively manipulate the proposed cause and measure the effect on the outcome variable.

  • Meta-analyses: These studies combine the results of multiple studies to provide a more comprehensive assessment of the evidence.

The stronger and more consistent the evidence, the more confidence we can have in the causal claim.

Importance of a Plausible Mechanism: How Does X Cause Y?

Finally, a strong causal claim should be supported by a plausible mechanism. This means that there should be a clear and understandable explanation of how the cause leads to the effect.

It’s not enough to simply observe that X and Y are related. You should also be able to explain why X causes Y.

For example, if you claim that a new drug reduces blood pressure, you should be able to explain the biological mechanisms through which the drug exerts its effect.

A plausible mechanism strengthens a causal claim by making it more believable and less susceptible to alternative explanations. Without a plausible mechanism, the relationship between X and Y might be due to chance or some other unknown factor.

Covariation, while essential, only paints a partial picture. We need to actively work to eliminate other potential explanations for the observed relationship. This quest for robust causal evidence often leads us to employ specific methodologies designed to isolate and validate causal links.

Methods for Investigating Causal Claims: Tools and Techniques

Establishing causality is not a passive endeavor. It requires active investigation, utilizing a range of tools and techniques designed to isolate and validate potential causal relationships. From controlled experiments to statistical analysis and philosophical frameworks, a multi-faceted approach is essential for building a convincing causal argument.

The Power of Experiments

Experiments stand as a cornerstone in the pursuit of causal knowledge. By carefully manipulating variables and controlling for extraneous factors, researchers can create artificial environments that allow them to isolate the impact of a specific cause on a specific effect. This active intervention is what distinguishes experiments from simple observational studies.

Control and Experimental Groups

At the heart of experimental design lies the comparison between a control group and an experimental group. The control group serves as a baseline, experiencing all conditions except for the variable being tested. The experimental group, on the other hand, is exposed to the proposed cause, allowing researchers to observe any resulting changes in the effect.

By comparing the outcomes in these two groups, researchers can gain valuable insights into whether the proposed cause has a genuine impact.

Randomized Controlled Trials (RCTs)

Among experimental methods, the Randomized Controlled Trial (RCT) holds a position of particular prominence. In an RCT, participants are randomly assigned to either the control group or the experimental group, ensuring that, at least statistically, the two groups are equivalent at the outset.

This randomization is crucial because it minimizes the influence of pre-existing differences between participants, strengthening the confidence in the observed causal relationship. RCTs are widely regarded as the "gold standard" for evaluating the effectiveness of interventions in fields like medicine and public health.

The Scientific Method: A Framework for Causal Inquiry

The scientific method provides a systematic framework for testing causal claims. This iterative process involves formulating a hypothesis, designing an experiment or study to test the hypothesis, collecting and analyzing data, and drawing conclusions based on the evidence.

When applied rigorously, the scientific method can help to identify potential causal relationships and rule out alternative explanations. The key is a commitment to objectivity, transparency, and a willingness to revise conclusions in the face of new evidence.

Statistical Significance: Interpreting the Data

Statistical analysis plays a crucial role in evaluating causal claims. Statistical significance refers to the probability that the observed results are not due to random chance. Researchers use p-values to quantify this probability.

A p-value of 0.05, for example, indicates that there is only a 5% chance that the observed results occurred randomly, suggesting that the relationship between the cause and effect is likely genuine.

Confidence intervals provide a range of values within which the true effect is likely to lie.

However, it’s crucial to remember that statistical significance does not automatically prove causation. It simply provides evidence that supports the existence of a relationship between the variables being studied.

Other factors, such as the study design, the presence of confounding variables, and the plausibility of the proposed mechanism, must also be considered.

John Stuart Mill’s Methods: A Foundation for Causal Analysis

In the 19th century, philosopher John Stuart Mill developed a set of methods for identifying causal relationships. These methods, while not without their limitations, continue to provide a valuable framework for causal analysis.

  • Method of Agreement: If two or more instances of a phenomenon have only one circumstance in common, that circumstance may be the cause of the phenomenon.
  • Method of Difference: If an instance in which the phenomenon occurs and an instance in which it does not occur have every circumstance in common save one, that one occurring only in the former, the circumstance in which alone the two instances differ is the effect, or the cause, or an indispensable part of the cause, of the phenomenon.
  • Method of Agreement and Difference (Joint Method): Combines the previous two methods to strengthen causal inference.
  • Method of Residues: Subtracting from any phenomenon such part as is known by previous inductions to be the effect of certain antecedents, the residue of the phenomenon is the effect of the remaining antecedents.
  • Method of Concomitant Variations: Whenever one phenomenon varies in any manner whenever another phenomenon varies in some particular manner, they are either causally connected or are connected through some fact of causation.

Mill’s methods provide a structured approach to identifying potential causal relationships based on patterns of co-occurrence and variation.

Common Fallacies in Causal Reasoning: Avoiding the Pitfalls

Establishing causality is a rigorous process, demanding careful consideration of evidence and potential alternative explanations. However, the path to uncovering true causal relationships is often fraught with logical pitfalls. These causal fallacies can lead to inaccurate conclusions, misinformed decisions, and ultimately, a flawed understanding of the world around us. Recognizing and avoiding these common errors in reasoning is paramount for anyone seeking to make sound judgments based on cause and effect.

The "Post Hoc Ergo Propter Hoc" Fallacy

One of the most prevalent causal fallacies is the "post hoc ergo propter hoc" fallacy, which translates to "after this, therefore because of this." This fallacy assumes that because one event follows another, the first event must have caused the second.

While temporal sequence is a necessary condition for causation (the cause must precede the effect), it is not a sufficient condition. Simply because event B happened after event A does not automatically mean that A caused B.

For example, imagine a scenario where a new CEO is appointed, and shortly thereafter, company profits increase. It would be fallacious to immediately conclude that the CEO’s appointment caused the increase in profits. Other factors, such as changes in market conditions, successful marketing campaigns launched before the appointment, or seasonal trends, could be responsible for the improved financial performance.

Ignoring Common Cause: The Lurking Variable

Another common pitfall in causal reasoning is ignoring the possibility of a common cause, also known as a confounding variable. This occurs when two events appear to be causally related, but in reality, both are caused by a third, unobserved factor. Failing to identify and account for these confounding variables can lead to erroneous causal conclusions.

Consider the example of a study that finds a correlation between ice cream sales and crime rates. It might be tempting to conclude that ice cream consumption causes crime, or vice versa. However, a more plausible explanation is that both ice cream sales and crime rates tend to increase during the summer months due to warmer weather. The temperature, in this case, is the confounding variable that influences both phenomena.

Reverse Causation: Mistaking Cause for Effect

Reverse causation is a fallacy that arises when the cause and effect are confused. In other words, it involves assuming that A causes B when, in reality, B causes A.

This type of error is particularly common in observational studies where the direction of the relationship is not immediately clear. For instance, consider the relationship between exercise and happiness.

While it might be tempting to assume that exercise causes happiness, it is also possible that happier people are more likely to exercise. In this case, happiness could be the cause, and exercise the effect, rather than the other way around.

The Importance of Avoiding Fallacious Reasoning

Avoiding these common causal fallacies is crucial for making sound decisions and forming accurate beliefs about the world. Fallacious reasoning can lead to ineffective policies, misguided strategies, and a general misunderstanding of complex phenomena.

By cultivating critical thinking skills, carefully evaluating evidence, and remaining vigilant against these logical traps, we can improve our ability to discern true causal relationships and make more informed judgments. The pursuit of causal knowledge demands intellectual rigor and a constant awareness of the potential for error. By acknowledging and addressing these common fallacies, we can navigate the complexities of causation with greater clarity and precision.

Necessary and Sufficient Conditions: Refining Causal Understanding

After identifying potential causal fallacies, we can further refine our understanding of causal relationships by exploring the concepts of necessary and sufficient conditions. These concepts provide a more nuanced framework for evaluating causal claims and determining the strength and validity of proposed cause-and-effect relationships. Grasping the difference between a condition that must be present for an effect to occur and one that guarantees the effect allows for more precise causal reasoning.

Understanding Necessary Conditions

A necessary condition is a condition that must be present for a particular effect or outcome to occur. If the necessary condition is absent, the effect cannot happen. In other words, Y cannot happen without X, therefore X is necessary for Y.

Think of it like this: oxygen is a necessary condition for a fire. You can have fuel and a spark, but without oxygen, combustion will not occur. The absence of oxygen guarantees the absence of fire.

However, it’s crucial to note that a necessary condition does not guarantee the effect will happen. Oxygen is necessary for fire, but oxygen alone will not cause a fire. Fuel and an ignition source are also required.

Defining Sufficient Conditions

A sufficient condition, on the other hand, is a condition that, if present, guarantees that a particular effect or outcome will occur. If X is sufficient for Y, then whenever X is present, Y will also be present.

For instance, decapitation is a sufficient condition for death. If a person is decapitated, death invariably follows.

However, a sufficient condition is not the only way for the effect to occur. There may be other ways to bring about the same effect. For example, while decapitation is sufficient for death, it’s not necessary. There are many other ways a person can die.

Strengthening or Weakening Causal Claims

Understanding the distinction between necessary and sufficient conditions is critical for evaluating and either strengthening or weakening causal claims.

If a proposed cause is shown to be a necessary condition for an effect, it strengthens the causal claim. It suggests that the cause plays a fundamental role in bringing about the effect. The more crucial that condition is, the stronger the argument for causation.

Conversely, if the proposed cause is shown not to be a necessary condition, it weakens the causal claim. It demonstrates that the effect can occur even in the absence of the proposed cause, suggesting that other factors may be at play.

If a proposed cause is shown to be a sufficient condition, it provides strong support for the causal claim. Every time the cause occurs, the effect follows, establishing a reliable relationship.

However, even if a proposed cause is not a sufficient condition, it doesn’t necessarily invalidate the causal claim. The cause may still be a contributing factor, even if it doesn’t guarantee the effect on its own.

By carefully analyzing whether a proposed cause is a necessary or sufficient condition (or neither), we can refine our understanding of causal relationships and avoid making overly simplistic or inaccurate causal claims. This added layer of scrutiny is key to sound reasoning and informed decision-making.

Examples of Causal Claims: Putting Theory into Practice

Having explored the theoretical underpinnings of causal claims, including necessary and sufficient conditions, it’s time to ground these concepts in real-world examples. Examining causal claims across diverse fields reveals the practical challenges and nuances involved in establishing valid causal relationships. These examples will showcase how to dissect a claim, identify its components, and assess its potential weaknesses.

Medical Causal Claims: The Case of Smoking and Lung Cancer

Perhaps one of the most well-known examples of a validated causal claim lies in the link between smoking and lung cancer. The claim asserts that smoking causes lung cancer.

Analyzing the Claim

The proposed cause is the act of smoking, specifically the inhalation of tobacco smoke. The effect is the development of lung cancer, a disease characterized by uncontrolled cell growth in the lungs.

Establishing Causality

The strength of this causal claim rests on decades of epidemiological and biological research. Studies have consistently demonstrated a strong statistical correlation between smoking and lung cancer. More importantly, research has established a plausible mechanism – the carcinogenic compounds in tobacco smoke damage lung cells, leading to mutations and, ultimately, cancer.

Addressing Potential Pitfalls

While the evidence strongly supports this claim, it’s crucial to acknowledge potential confounding variables. Not everyone who smokes develops lung cancer, and some individuals who have never smoked do.

Genetic predisposition, exposure to other carcinogens (like radon), and pre-existing lung conditions can also contribute to the development of lung cancer.

However, the overwhelming evidence indicates that smoking significantly increases the risk of lung cancer, making it a robust causal claim.

Economic Causal Claims: Interest Rates and Inflation

In economics, causal claims are often used to explain and predict market behavior. A common claim is that raising interest rates reduces inflation.

Analyzing the Claim

Here, the proposed cause is an increase in interest rates by a central bank. The anticipated effect is a decrease in the rate of inflation, which is the general increase in prices of goods and services in an economy.

Exploring the Mechanism

The underlying mechanism is that higher interest rates make borrowing more expensive. This, in turn, reduces consumer spending and business investment, decreasing overall demand in the economy.

With less demand, businesses are less likely to raise prices, thus curbing inflation.

Identifying Complexities

It is a highly debated claim.

The relationship between interest rates and inflation is complex and influenced by numerous other factors, such as global supply chains, government fiscal policies, and consumer confidence.

Simply raising interest rates may not always be sufficient to control inflation, especially if other factors are exerting strong upward pressure on prices.

Furthermore, the effect of interest rate changes can take time to materialize, making it challenging to isolate the specific impact of this one variable.

Social Science Causal Claims: Education and Income

Causal claims in the social sciences often explore the relationships between various social factors. A frequently discussed claim is that higher levels of education lead to higher income.

Deconstructing the Claim

The proposed cause is increased educational attainment, typically measured by years of schooling or degrees earned. The predicted effect is higher individual income, often measured annually or over a lifetime.

Examining the Connection

The presumed mechanism is that education equips individuals with valuable skills, knowledge, and credentials that make them more productive and competitive in the labor market. This increased productivity translates into higher wages and better career opportunities.

Acknowledging Limitations

While a positive correlation between education and income is well-established, it is important to acknowledge that correlation is not causation.

Other factors, such as socioeconomic background, innate abilities, and networking opportunities, also play significant roles in determining income.

Additionally, the quality of education matters. A degree from a prestigious institution may open more doors than a degree from a less-regarded one.

Therefore, while education is likely a contributing factor to higher income, it is not the sole determinant, and the relationship is far from simple.

Importance of Context and Critical Evaluation

These examples illustrate that establishing causal claims requires careful analysis, consideration of potential confounding variables, and a thorough understanding of the underlying mechanisms. Each field has its own unique challenges and complexities. By critically evaluating causal claims and considering the evidence and reasoning behind them, we can make more informed decisions and avoid falling prey to faulty logic.

Frequently Asked Questions About Causal Claims

These FAQs clarify some common questions about causal claims and how to identify them.

What exactly is a causal claim definition?

A causal claim definition states that one thing directly causes another. It’s a statement asserting a cause-and-effect relationship. For example, "Eating processed foods causes weight gain" is a causal claim.

How can I distinguish a causal claim from a correlation?

Correlation means two things are related, but not necessarily that one causes the other. Causation implies a direct influence. Just because events happen together doesn’t mean one caused the other. Always look for evidence to support the causal claim definition.

What evidence supports a strong causal claim?

Strong evidence involves factors like repeated experiments with consistent results, a plausible mechanism explaining how the cause leads to the effect, and controlling for other potential contributing factors. The stronger the evidence, the more reliable the causal claim definition.

Why is understanding causal claims important?

Recognizing and evaluating causal claims is crucial for making informed decisions in everyday life. From health to finance, understanding whether a cause-and-effect relationship is valid helps you avoid making choices based on misleading or inaccurate information. Having a solid causal claim definition lets you do that.

Hopefully, this simple guide clarifies what a causal claim definition actually means. Go out there and question those assumptions – the world is full of them! Thanks for reading, and good luck sorting through all those causal claims!

Related Posts

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *